New technique helps doctors more accurately detect autism in young children
The Sector > Research > Understanding Children > New technique helps doctors more accurately detect autism in young children

New technique helps doctors more accurately detect autism in young children

by Freya Lucas

July 10, 2019

Researchers have developed a new technique which supports doctors to more quickly and accurately detect autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in children.

 

In a study led by the University of Waterloo, researchers characterised how children with ASD scan a person’s face differently than a neuro-typical child. Based on the findings, the researchers were able to develop a technique that considers how a child with ASD transitions gaze from one part of a person’s face to another.

 

According to the developers, the use of this technology makes the diagnostic process less stressful for the children and, if combined with existing manual methods, could help doctors better avoid a false positive autism diagnosis. 

 

A diagnosis of ASD can mean that young children attending early childhood education and care (ECEC) settings may face challenges such as: communication difficulties, differing sensory needs, a strong need for routine and predictability, struggling to cope with change, issues with toileting, problems with eating, intense focus on a narrow field of interest, becoming overwhelmed and having subsequent “melt downs”, and play in the traditional sense can be challenging. 

 

Research leader Mehrshad Sadria said early diagnosis was especially important in ensuring good outcomes for children living with ASD. 

 

“The current approaches to determining if someone has autism are not really child-friendly. Our method allows for the diagnosis to be made more easily and with less possibility of mistakes,” Mr Sadria said. 

 

“The new technique can be used in all ASD diagnosis, but we believe it’s particularly effective for children.”

 

In developing the new technique, the researchers evaluated 17 children with ASD and 23 neuro-typical children. The mean chronological ages of the ASD and neuro-typical groups were 5.5 years  and 4.8 years, respectively.

 

Each participant was shown 44 photographs of faces on a 19-inch screen, integrated into an eye-tracking system. The infrared device interpreted and identified the locations on the stimuli at which each child was looking via emission and reflection of wave from the iris.

 

The images were separated into seven key areas of interest (AOIs) in which participants focussed their gaze: under the right eye, right eye, under the left eye, left eye, nose, mouth and other parts of the screen. 

 

The researchers wanted to know more than how much time the participants spent looking at each AOI, but also how they moved their eyes and scan the faces. To get that information, the researchers used four different concepts from network analysis to evaluate the varying degree of importance the children placed on the seven AOIs when exploring the facial features.

 

The first concept determined the number of other AOIs that the participant directly moves their eyes to and from a particular AOI. The second concept looked at how often a particular AOI is involved when the participant moves their eyes between two other AOIs as quickly as possible. The third concept is related to how quickly one can move their eyes from a particular AOI to other AOIs. The fourth concept measured the importance of an AOI, in the context of eye movement and face scanning, by the number of important AOIs that it shares direct transitions with.

 

Currently, the two most favoured ways of assessing ASD involve a questionnaire or an evaluation from a psychologist.

 

“It is much easier for children to just look at something, like the animated face of a dog, than to fill out a questionnaire or be evaluated by a psychologist,” said Anita Layton, who supervised Mr Sadria’s research, and is a professor of Applied Mathematics, Pharmacy and Biology at Waterloo. 

 

“Also, the challenge many psychologists face is that sometimes behaviours deteriorate over time, so the child might not display signs of autism, but then a few years later, something starts showing up. 

 

“Our technique is not just about behaviour or whether a child is focussing on the mouth or eyes. It’s about how a child looks at everything.” 

 

The study, Network Centrality Analysis of Eye-gaze Data in Autism Spectrum Disorder, was recently published in the journal Computers in Biology and Medicine.

Download The Sector's new App!

ECEC news, jobs, events and more anytime, anywhere.

Download App on Apple App Store Button Download App on Google Play Store Button
PRINT